[home]
last updated:

April 25, 2025

[Brain Image]    

PSY 340 Brain and Behavior

Class 38: Evolution and Physiology of Language

   

All living creatures communicate in some fashion using sound, sight, motions, touch, odors etc. as means of signalling.

Animal
          Communication


The Evolution and Physiology of Language

A.   Compared to all other forms of animal communication, human language is unique because of it productivity, that is, its ability to produce new signals to represent new ideas.

Human language is distinct from all other known animal forms of communication in being compositional. Human language allows speakers to express thoughts in sentences comprising subjects, verbs and objects—such as ‘I kicked the ball’—and recognizing past, present and future tenses. Compositionality gives human language an endless capacity for generating new sentences as speakers combine and recombine sets of words into their subject, verb and object roles. For instance, with just 25 different words for each role, it is already possible to generate over 15,000 distinct sentences. Human language is also referential, meaning speakers use it to exchange specific information with each other about people or objects and their locations or actions. (Pagel, 2017)

Consider how the invention of the Internet has forced us to create a whole new set of symbols and ways of conveying meanings.

Internet Abbreviations  emojis

B.  Nonhuman Precursors to Language

[Chimpanzee]1.  Common chimpanzees can not learn to talk, but can learn some language skills using American Sign Language or other visual systems. Their use of language-related symbols differ from human language in many ways:

a. The chimpanzees seldom used the symbols in new original combinations (they are not productive).
b. The chimpanzees used their symbols almost always to make a request, only rarely to describe.
c. The chimpanzees produced requests far better than they seem to understand anyone else's request.
d. They do show a moderate degree of understanding of what is communicated to them, e.g., "who?" questions answered by names; "what?" questions answered by things; and "where?" questions answered by places.

[Bonobo]2. Bonobos (Pan paniscus) , a "cousin" of the common chimpanzee, when given language training uses symbols in several ways that more resemble humans than common chimpanzees:

a.  They understood more information than they produce.
b.  They use symbols to name and describe objects even when they are not requesting them.
c.  They request items that they do not see.
d.  They occasionally use the symbols to describe past events.
e.  They frequently make original, creative requests.

3.  The reason for the better language skills in the bonobos than in chimps is unknown.

4.  Nonprimates: Alex, African gray parrot (1976-2007) 

Trained by Dr. Irene Pepperberg (originally U Arizona, now affiliated with Brandeis U-Harvard-visiting @ MIT Media Lab) from age 1. Alex died unexpectedly Sept 6, 2007 of heart disease.
 
Alex had relatively extensive language ability with specific objects & concepts:[Alex the Parrot &
                Irene Pepperberg]
  • Alex "could identify fifty different objects and recognize quantities up to six; that he could distinguish seven colors and five shapes, and understand the concepts of "bigger", "smaller", "same", and "different," and that he was learning "over" and "under".
     
  • Alex had a vocabulary of about 150 words, but was exceptional in that he appeared to have understanding of what he said. For example, when Alex was shown an object and was asked about its shape, color, or material, he could label it correctly. If asked the difference between two objects, he also answered that, but if there was no difference between the objects, he said “none.”" (Wikipedia, retrieved April 21, 2008)
Pepperberg offers an evolutionary explanation for intelligence (which, presumably, is the basis of language) by citing the examples of both apes and parrots who live to become 30 to 60 years old in captivity (but parrots average about 24 years in the wild):

"Nick Humphrey suggested these ideas almost 30 years ago:... given a long-lived creature that exists in a complex socio-ecological system, that creature has likely been selected for high-level intelligence and cognition. I think those same evolutionary pressures work on parrots." ("That damned bird," 2003)

C.  How Did Humans Evolve Language?

  1.  Is Language Just a Product of Overall Intelligence or a Specialized Adaptation? Answer: Probably a Specialized Adaptation

Brain-Body
            Ratioa. The relationship between brain and brain-to-body ratio is unclear (see chart  or click here for larger image). Human beings do not have the largest brain-to-body mass ratio: dolphins, elephants, and blue whales do. Yet these animals do no use language in ways that are parallel to human beings.

b. The evolution of language seems to require a brain mechanism called the phonological loop, that is, the ability to hear and remember something (remember that this loop is one of the elements in Baddeley's model of working memory).

Language also seems to depend upon gestures, particularly those gestures involving the face and mouth. When we listen to others in noisy settings, we pay particular attention to the face and mouth of the other speaker in order to understand what they are saying.

[KE Family
            Tree]c. People with Normal Intelligence but Impaired Language

"KE" Family (an English Caucasian family; for many years was mistakenly described as of Pakistani origin) & "CS" (an unrelated English boy with same language problem)

Presumably because of an altered dominant gene (FOXP2 ["“forkhead box P2”"] segment on chromosome 7), 16 of 30 people of normal intelligence within one family (& "CS") have severe difficulty with pronunciation, and all other aspects of language. Cases such as this suggest that genetic conditions which affect brain development can impair language without impacting other aspects of intelligence. (Itzhaki, 2003; Liegeois et al., 2003; MacAndrew, 2003).

==> Conclusion 1: General intelligence is not itself sufficient for language.

d. People with Intellectual Disability (Low Intelligence) but Relatively Spared Language

Williams syndrome (also known as Williams-Beuren Syndrome) = people with intellectual disability but good language skills. Also musical rhythm ability. Fascination with faces (fusiform gyrus is 2X normal).

Williams Person   Williams Syndrome FFA 

A rare disorder (~1 in 20,000 births according to Kalat; ~1 in 8000 live births according to Haas & Reiss, 2012) in which individuals with intellectual impairments have relatively skillful use of language, but limited abilities in other regards. This disorder is caused by a deletion of several genes from chromosome 7. 

Pascual-Castroviejo et al. (2004) summarized other characteristic symptoms:

New/Not in book:

Ev
          FedorenkoEvelina Fedorenko (MGH/Harvard) & Rosemary Varley (UCL, UK; 2016) report that "language and thought are not the same thing" on the basis of multiple neuroimaging studies
Conclusion 2: Language does not appear to be a by-product of general intelligence or general intellectual activity

2.  Is Language a Specialization of the Brain?

Noam Chomsky @
              2017 APA Washington DCa.  First proposed by MIT linguistics scholar, Noam Chomsky, an alternate view of the evolution of language is that language evolved as an extra brain module, called a language acquisition device (LAD).  This idea is supported by the fact that children learn language with amazing ease and that children learn language despite the fact they do not hear enough examples to learn the grammatical structure of language (this is called the poverty of the stimulus argument).

Chomsky believes that language did not primarily develop as a method of vocal communication, but a means by which to organize our thoughts internally. In so doing, we developed a particularly important new skill: "symbolic thinking". As Ian Tattersall (2024) summarizes

And it is certainly true that no other human attribute maps better than language does onto the “symbolic” way in which human beings process information in their minds. Uniquely, as far as we can tell, we humans deconstruct our interior and exterior worlds into a vocabulary of discrete mental symbols. Once we have done this, we can shuffle those symbols around, according to rules, to produce statements not only about those worlds as they are but as they might be.

b. A Sensitive Period for Language Learning

Argument: Language has a critical period, because if you don't learn language when you are young, you will forever be language disadvantaged.

Conclusion 3: There is no language module which automatically causes a person to learn to speak. Rather the predisposing neural structures require social experience in the company of other people in order to develop most appropriately.

3. Language and the "Social Brain" Hypothesis (not in textbook)

a. [Robin
              Dunbar]Without rejecting the LAD theory above, we should note that language always arises within a social context and depends upon social interaction for its development (a corollary of the "critical period" observation). A purely biological explanation for the emergence of language is unlikely. Vasanta (2005) states this caution by claiming:

"...syntax-centered definitions of language knowledge [such as the LAD theory] completely ignore certain crucial aspects of language learning and use, namely, that language is embedded in a social context; that the role of environmental triggering as a learning mechanism is grossly underestimated; that a considerable extent of visuo-spatial information accompanies speech in day-to-day communication; that the developmental process itself lies at the heart of knowledge acquisition; and that there is a tremendous variation in the orthographic systems associated with different languages." (Abstract)

b. Contrary to the theory of the evolution of general intelligence, Robin Dunbar (U Liverpool, UK) and others propose that language developed as a way of helping organize larger and larger social grouping. This "social brain" hypothesis rests upon a variety of observations including two advantages conveyed by language:

  • the ability to categorize individuals into distinct types (e.g., doctor, sheriff, chief)
  • the ability to instruct other individuals about how they should respond "toward specific types of individuals within society" (Dunban, 1993)

Essentially, language permits societies of much larger size than would be possible otherwise. Such groups, e.g., bands, tribes, etc., have an survival advantage vis-a-vis those who do not have language.

c. Theory of Mind, Mental Time Travel, & Narrative as Factors in the Evolution of Language

Mental Time
              Travel and Narrative


D. When did human language evolve/develop?

Early H.
          sapiens in Africa (Timbrell, 2024)   H. sapiens migrations out of Africa

A recent estimate of when our modern human species (Homo sapiens) evolved from earlier primate and hominid ancestors (Vidal et al., 2022) points to Eastern Africa 230 kya (+/- 22 kya; kya = "thousands of years ago"). Note in the map of Africa on the left above (Timbrell, 2024), the densest sites for tools and other human artifacts from the prehistoric period are found along the eastern side of the continent, particularly from the northeastern Ethiopian Highlands through the central Great Rift Valley down toward the southeastern hills of South Africa. Most archeologists of prehistory argue that H. sapiens originally emerged in the Great Rift Valley. Over the next 100-150 thousand years, H. sapiens migrated from Eastern Africa to spread throughout the rest of Africa, Asia, Europe, and Australia (I am omitting the migrations from Asia to the Americas which occurred ~20-15 kya)

According to estimates there are approximately 7,100 different languages spoken across the Earth among the 8 billion people populating the world.

Can we go backward in time to understand when human language first evolved or developed? The task is difficult:

“Since the emergence of language lies so far back in human prehistory, the relevant developments have left no direct historical traces, and comparable processes cannot be observed today” (https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Origin_of_language, April 18, 2025)

Nonetheless, it is notable that among all the languages spoken today, they "share striking similarities in the ways in which they are constructed phonologically, syntactically, and semantically" (Miyagawa et al. 2025 citing Eberhard et al. 2023). This suggests that despite so many different language there must be a common neurological and biological origin for human language itself. Thus, a strategy to detect when language developed would be to discover when the original Homo sapiens (modern humans) were last centered together in a common grouping, that is, before humans split apart and began to spread across the continents.

In order to determine when humans were last together as a common group, Miyagawa et al. (2025) used genetic markers from whole genomes of African populations (including Y chromosomes and mitochondrial DNA markers) to pinpoint when the first divergence in human populations occurred (it is known as the "Khosian divergence event"). The conclusion of their research states: "These genomic studies of early H. sapiens suggest that linguistic  potential must have been present in the H. sapiens population at the latest by 135kya" (p. 3; kya = thousand years ago). Miyagawa et al. (2025) elaborated on the meaning of their findings:

Based on the recent genetic studies of early H. sapiens, we have pinpointed approximately 135kya as the moment at which some linguistic capacity must have been present in the human population. Looking forward from this event, modern human behaviors such as body decoration and the production of ochre pieces with symbolic engravings appeared as normative and persistent behaviors around 100kya. We  believe that the time lag implied between the lower boundary of when language was present (135kya) and the emergence of normative modern human behaviors across the population suggests that language itself was the trigger that transformed nonlinguistic early H. sapiens (who nonetheless already possessed “language-ready” brains acquired at the origin of the anatomically distinctive species) into the symbolically-mediated beings familiar today. This development of the most sophisticated communication device in evolution allowed our ancestors to accelerate and consolidate symbolically-mediated behaviors until they became the norm for the entire species. (p. 4; emphases added).

Hence, we might conclude as they did that by 135,000 years ago, humans were "language ready" and over the subsequent tens of thousands of years actual languages emerged which expanded all of our behaviors that use symbols, the principal characteristic of the human species.

E. Strengths associated with human language.

Linguist and psychologist, Julie Sedivy (2024) points out three major strengths that humans achieve through our abilities to use language:

1. Language allows us to align our minds with those of others and represents a powerful method by which we can achieve connection with others.

2. Much of our experience of language comes from the intimate sensory settings in which we first learned words and concepts. Infants and young learn the sounds and words of their native language much more easily from those they know than from videos or other non-personal means. We acquire sensory memories and experiences that are tied up with the words we use.

3. Our linguistic abilities developed over a lifetime help us to overcome some of the limitations that come with the advances of old age (hearing loss, illness, stroke, etc.)

F. Brain Damage and Language

  1. Aphasia = Severe acquired language impairment due to damage to the brain's language areas (for most people in the left hemisphere)

Causes of aphasia include

 2a. Broca's Area: Small part of the frontal lobe of the left cerebral cortex that when damaged leads to impairments in language production.

  2b. Wernicke's Area

Binder 2017      Aphasias

  3. Broca's aphasia (or nonfluent aphasia): A language impairment whose most prominent symptom is a deficit in language production. Caused by damage to Broca' area and surrounding areas.

a. Patients suffering from Broca's aphasia often speak meaningfully, but omit pronouns, prepositions, conjunctions, and qualifiers from their own speech; they also have trouble understanding these same kinds of words. Other more seriously impaired individuals have significant difficulty forming the words themselves and will slur or otherwise produce difficult to understand language.

YouTube:   Broca's aphasia - Sarah Scott - teenage stroke

  4. Wernicke's aphasia or fluent aphasia involves a difficulty in comprehending the verbal and written communications of others.  Although patients can still speak smoothly, their speech content is often nonsensical.  They also have anomia (difficulty recalling the names of objects). Note above the damage causing this may have nothing to do with "Wernicke's Area"

YouTube:Wernicke's aphasia - Retired dentist

YouTube icon Wernicke's aphasia - Young woman (moderately impaired

  5. Language requires the activation of many different areas other than the frontal cortex (Broca's area and surrounding regions) and the temporal cortex.

     E. Dyslexia

1. Dyslexia: Inability to read or significant difficulty with reading despite adequate vision and intelligence.  Many kinds of dyslexia exist with different underlying causes. This is also a topic which has caused a great deal of argumentation and struggle among researchers, teachers, parents, and students for many decades.

Dyslexic
              brain2. D'Mello & Gabrieli (2018) summarize the key findings from fMRI and other studies. These suggest that individuals with dyslexia show....

3.  As a rule, a dyslexic person is more likely to have a bilaterally symmetrical cerebral cortex (i.e., the planum temporale and other structures are the same size on the left and right hemisphere).

4.  Other problems observed in persons with dyslexia (note that not all individuals with dyslexia shows all these problems):

a. Dysphonic dyslexia (dys = poor; phonos = sound) = difficulty sounding out printed words

b. Dyseidetic dyslexia (dys = poor; eidos = image) = difficulty recognizing words as a whole (though can sound them out)

Fixation Dyslexia Readingc. Poor auditory memory: difficulty in remembering the sequence of sounds. Suggests that there may be a problem involving the connection between the visual and auditory processing areas of the brain.

c. Dyslexia is a function of attentional differences in which there are problems attending to information directly in front of the reader, that is, dyslexic readers have a fixation point 5 to 10 degrees to the right or left of the word in front of them. Check out the image on the right side.

Try to keep your eye fixated on the dot in the middle while you try to read the letters on either the right or the left of the dot. Individuals with dyslexia often find it easier to do so than those who do not have dyslexia.



References

Binder, J. R. (2017). Current controversies on Wernicke’s Area and its role in language. Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 17, 58. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-017-0764-8

DeWitt, I., & Rauschecker, J. P. (2012) Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. PNAS. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113427109

D'Mello, A. M., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2019). Cognitive neuroscience of dyslexia. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 49, 798-809. doi: 10.1044/2018_LSHSS-DYSLC-18-0020

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681-735. [Full-text]

Dunbar, R. I. M. (1998). The social brain hypothesis. Evolutionary Anthropology, 6, 178-190. [pdf version]

Eberhard, D. M., Simons, G. F., & Fennig, C. D. (2023). Ethnologue: Languages of the world (26e). Dallas, TX: SIL International.

Fedorenko, E., & Varley, R. (2016). Language and thought are not the same thing: Evidence from neuroimaging and neurological patients. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. doi: 10.1111/nyas.13046

Haas, B. W., & Reiss, A. L. (2012). Social brain development in Williams syndrome: The current status and directions for future research. Frontiers in Psychology. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00186

Itzhaki, J. (2003, April 28). The FOXP2 story: A single family with speech abormalities may hold one of the keys to the origin of human culture. Retrieved April 19, 2009 from The Human Genome website (Wellcome Trust: London, UK).

Jerison, H. J. (1973). Evolution of the brain and intelligence. New York: Academic Press.

Liégeois, F., Baldewegl, T., Connelly, A., Gadian, D. G., Mishkin, M., & Vargha-Khademl, F. (2003). Language fMRI abnormalities associated with FOXP2 gene mutation. Nature Neuroscience, 6, 1230-1237.

Mantell, J. T., & Pfordresher, P. Q. (2013). Vocal imitation of song and speech. Cognition, 127, 177-202. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2012.12.008

McAndrew, A. (2003). FOXP2 and the Evolution of Language. Downloaded 4/20/05 from the Web site: http://www.evolutionpages.com/FOXP2_language.htm

Miyagawa, S., DeSalle, R., Nóbrega, V. A., Nitscheke, R., Okumura, M.,  Tattersall, I. (2025). Linguistic capacity was present in the Homo sapiens population 135 thousand years ago. Frontiers in Psychology, 16, 1503900. https://dx.doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2025.1503900

Mithen, S. (2024). The language puzzle: Piecing together the six-million-year story of how words evolved. Basic Books.

NINDS Dyslexia Information Page (2005, February). Bethesda, MD: National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke (NINDS). Downloaded from the NINDS Web site: http://www.ninds.nih.gov/disorders/dyslexia/dyslexia.htm

Pagel, M. (2017). Q&A: What is human language, when did it evolve and why should we care. BMC Biology, 15, article 64. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12915-017-0405-3

Pascual-Castroviejo, I., Pascual-Pascual, S. I., Moreno Granado, F., Garcia-Guereta, L, Gracia-Bouthelier, R., Navarro Torres, M., Delicado Navarro, A., Lopez-Pajares, D., & Palencia Luaces, R. (2004). Síndrome de Williams-Beuren: Presentación de 82 casos [Williams-Beuren syndrome: Presentation of 82 cases]. Anales de Pédiatria (Barcelona), 60(6), 530-536. [PubMed Abstract] [Full-text Spanish]

Pinker, S. (1996). Language acquisition. Downloaded 4/20/05 from the Web site: http://www.ecs.soton.ac.uk/~harnad/Papers/Py104/pinker.langacq.html

Porter, M. A., & Coltheart, M. (2005). Cognitive heterogeneity in Williams syndrome. Developmental Neuropsychology, 27(2), 275-306. [PubMed Abstract]

Sedivy, J. (2024, October 15). What language reveals a out us. Nautilus [Online]. https://nautil.us/what-language-reveals-about-us-967370/

Tattersall, I. (2024, December 19). Look who’s talking [Review of The language puzzle: Piecing together the six-million-year story of how words evolved by Steven Mithen]. New York Review of Books.

Temple, E., Deutsch, G. K., Poldrack, R. A., Miller, S. L., Tallal, P., Merzenich, M. M., Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2003). Neural deficits in children with dyslexia ameliorated by behavioral remediation: Evidence from functional MRI. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 100(5), 2860-2865. [PubMed Abstract] [Full-text]

That damn bird: A talk with Irene Pepperberg (2003, September 23). Edge Foundation. Downloaded from the Edge Foundation Web site: http://www.edge.org/3rd_culture/pepperberg03/pepperberg_index.html

Timbrell, L. (2024). Ecology and demography of early Homo sapiens: A synthesis of archaeological and climactic data from eastern Africa. Azania: Archeological Research in Africa, 59(1), 76-110. https://doi.org/10.1080/0067270X.2024.2307790

Vasanta, D. (2005). Language cannot be reduced to biology: Perspectives from neuro-developmental disorders affecting language learning. Journal of Bioscience, 30(1), 129-137. [PubMed Abstract]

Vidal, C. M., Lane, C. S., Asrat, A., et al. (2022) Age of the oldest known Homo sapiens from eastern Africa. Nature, 601, 579-583. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586-021-04275-8

Woodpigeon. (2000). Feral children. Entry A269840 at H2G2. BBC. Retrieved April 21, 2004 from the BBC Website: http://www.bbc.co.uk/dna/h2g2/alabaster/A269840/   


The first version of this page was posted on April 24, 2005