[home]
last updated:


February 11, 2024

[Brain Image]    

PSY 340 Brain and Behavior

Class 13: Research Methods in Neuropsychology

   

Note: The material in this class wlll not appear on Test #1, but on Test #2.  


The
Problem Solved by Korbinian Brodmann (1868-1918)

Korbinian
              BrodmannThe rapid development of neurology and psychiatry in the second half of the 19th and early part of the 20th century presented researchers with a significant problem in communication. There were agreed-upon terms for the major lobes and gyri of the human cerebral cortex. However, there remained a difficulty in identifying subregions of the cortex in a way which would be understood by scientists in different nations who spoke different languages. The work of Korbinian Brodmann helped to settle that problem.

Brodmann examined the cellular and laminar structure of the human cortex and the cortical tissue of other animals. Eventually he published his important monograph on the cytoarchitectonic structure of the human cortex in 1909. "Cytoarchitectonic" means the architecture of the cells (cyto = cell in Greek) Dr. Laurence Garey (1994) notes:

The basis of Brodmann's cortical localisation is its subdivision into 'areas' with similar cellular and laminar structure. He compared localisation in the human cortex with that in a number of other mammals, including primates, rodents and marsupials. In humans, he distinguished 47 areas, each carrying an individual number, and some being further subdivided.

Brodmann's numbering of these cortical locations has become one of the standard ways in which clinician identify brain areas. These are generally known as "Brodmann Areas" (BA) and will often be cited in texts, for example, as "in BA 45 and 46...". It is presumed that the informed reader will know already or have access to a map of these areas (see below):

Brodmann's
              Original Map
Brodmann Areas of
            the Lobes of the Left Hemisphere







How do we learn about how the brain works?

In one of my classes a while ago, a student asked, "So, how do we know these things about the brain?" Indeed, how do we know that the occipital lobe deals with vision and the frontal lobe with working memory? Why can we say that the hippocampus is responsible for any new declarative memories, but NOT for procedural memories or learning like how to ride a bike?

Brain vs. Behaviors

1. Look at the Effects of Brain Damage

2. Stimulate Some Brain Area and Analyze the Resulting Behavioral Change

3. Correlate Brain Anatomy with Behavior

4. Record Brain Activity during Behavior




1. Look at the Effects of Brain Damage

   

broca_tanPaul Pierre Broca (1824-1880) 
  • Patient could only say, "tan, tan" after stroke. 
  • Autopsy of brain (see left) found damage to lateral posterior (side, rear area) of frontal lobe.
  • Broca's or expressive aphasia: inability or difficulty speaking clear language.

Stereotaxic Instrument

Animal Brain Research


NOTE: Until the advent of modern technologies [described below] in the late 20th and early 21st centuries, it was not possible to examine directly brain damage in a living human being. As Broca's work showed, brain damage could only be seen at autopsy after the death of a patient.


transcranial magnetic stimulation
Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation (TMS)




2. Stimulate Some Brain Area and Analyze the Resulting Behavioral Change

In the 19th & early 20th centuries, physiologists began to stimulate the brains of experimental animals (dogs in particular) by using an electrical stimulus. This research established that some important areas of the brain were responsible for certain sensory experiences and motor skills.

Rat (Optogenetics)Optogenetics = Using light to control a limited group of neurons




3. Correlate Brain Anatomy with Behavior

Computerized Tomography (CT) (formerly called Computerized Axial Tomography (CAT)

CAT/CT ScanCAT Scan -
            hemorrhage

Images will show areas of damage. For example, in the CAT scan image on the right, the individual has experienced a cerebral hemorrhage (a type of stroke). Indeed, in most emergency rooms, if a patient is brought in with a suspected stroke or other brain injury, the first type of imaging they will receive is a CT scan.

Magnetic Resonance Imaging (MRI)

MRIMRI Brain Tumor

In the MRI image on the right, the patient appears to have a large tumor growing in the medial surface of the cortex of the brain (the posterior of the frontal lobe). This has compressed the cingulate cortex and is pressing down on the corpus callosum.



4. Recording Brain Activity during Behavior


[EEG of Child with Petit Mal Epilepsy]EEG (Electroencephalograph)


[EEG 1929]



PET Scan (Positron Emission Tomography)

PET Scan of Body
A person is injected with a slightly radioactive tracer linked to the molecule glucose (sugar). Brain (or body) tissues that are most active will use the most glucose and after a short while a lot of glucose will accumulate in active brain (or body) tissues. A by-product of radioactive decay of the tracer attached to the glucose molecule is a subatomic positron (or “antielectron”) particle which soon interacts with a nearby subatomic electron particle. This collision causes the annihilation of both particles and the emission of of two “gamma photons” which move in a straight line in opposite directions. Radiation detectors (scintillators) detect the near simultaneous arrival of the photons and, thus, can pinpoint where in the body or brain the glucose was used most heavily. [Animated image on right from https://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/File:PET-MIPS-anim.gif]

Pet Scanner Diagram  PET
        scan of temporal lobe bilaterally

Functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging (fMRI)

fMRI of
            occipital lobeRegular MRIs (see above) tell us about the structure of the brain. In the last decade, though, functional MRI (fMRI) scanners can tell us about the functioning or activity of the brain. The scanners detect where blood is being used by focusing upon the hemoglobin molecule which gives up oxygen. This is called looking for the BOLD (= Blood Oxygen Level Dependent) signal.

In an fMRI scan, a baseline structural image (MRI) of the brain is taken. Then, the brain is scanned (1) when it is not doing any task (Scan 1) and then (2) when it IS doing a specific task or activity (Scan 2). Scan 1 is subtracted from Scan 2 in order to identify those areas which are particularly active during the task.

The image to the left shows the brain as an individual is focusing upon a complex moving visual image. The areas highlighted in yellow and orange represent greater levels of activity compared to when the individual is focused upon a blank screen.

Taken from http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/Image:FMRI.jpg

Default Mode NetworkExamples of psychological findings from fMRI

Head Movement: A major difficulty with fMRI imaging is the problem of head movement. Even very small motions can cause the wrong areas of the brain to display when someone is doing a task.

Limited Resolution Scale: Another major concern is the impossibility (at least now) to capture the activity of just a small number of neurons (that is, having very high and focused resolution) rather than the functions of many thousands of neurons together. Recent estimates note that cubic millimeter of cortical tissue contains anywhere from 50,000 to 100,000 neurons (e.g., see Shapson-Coe et al. 2021). Until recently, many fMRI studies could only "see" the activity of a volume of brain tissue measuring 3 cubic millimeters. That means, the joint activity of a million or more neurons being measured as a group is the best resolution scale in many past studies. Newer fMRI equipment with much higher magnetic fields are now emerging and the resolution scale is improving.


Emerging Techniques. Note that there are a host of new techniques of imaging or studying the brain which we will not review and which you will not be responsible for knowing. These include CLARITY Imaging of Tissue (Postmortem), Magnetoencephalography, Magnetic Resonance Spectroscopy, Near Infrared Spectroscopy, Syringe-Injectable Electronics, and Ultrasound Neuroimaging. For anyone in the class who expects to enter a career in neuroscience or neuro-related medicine and health care, these kinds of techniques will doubtless become more and more important in the decades ahead.




Brain vs. Body MassBrain Size and Intelligence

1. Humans do not have the largest brains across animal species. We do not even have the largest ratio between brain size & body size.

2. Animals with larger brains often have larger neurons so that comparing the volume of animal with human brains is not equivalent.

3. Humans: Brains vs. IQ (Intelligence)
  • There is a moderate correlation between brain size and IQ tests (r = ~.30-40, which equals explaining about 9% to 16% of IQ differences due to size).
  • CAUTION: IQ test scores may not be adequate to evaluate "intelligence" which is a notoriously difficult concept to quantify.
    • Hevern: Intelligence involves those abilities to cope successfully within whatever environments (physical, interpersonal, or cultural) individuals find themselves.
    • Because IQ tests only measure a range of cognitive functions appropriate for life in an industrialized or economically-developed world, they are not necessarily measures of the full range of coping abilities (=intelligence) that are associated with differing environments.
  • Males generally do have larger brains than females (roughly 8-10%). However, there are no overall IQ differences between men and women.
  • Males vs. Females
    • Differences of specific skills or ability levels between men and women are sometimes found. However, within the context of differing developmental pathways (boys and girls often experience growing up with different opportunities and emphases), many such differences are not biological, but actually cultural, e.g., the superiority of boys in math may reflect the opportunities boys take to take part in activities involving numbers or geometric shapes.
    • There are differences in the amount of gray vs. white matter across the sexes. Women have deeper & more sulci than men and, despite females brains being roughly 8-10% smaller in volume, there is equal cortical surface for men and women.
    • Some structural differences have been found in the wiring between female and male brains. The significance of these findings is not clear.
    • And, when such structural differences are found, it is not immediately clear that there is any relationship between such structural differences and actual behavioral outcome. For example, Liu et al. (2020) reported that males have a larger face processing cortical area than females; yet, studies such as Rennels & Cummings (2013) affirm that women do better than men in actual face processing tasks.
  • Is there a sex/gender (s/g) difference in the human brain? Except for an overall size difference as noted above, comprehensive meta-analyses of sex/gender brain differences and their relationship to actual behavior shows very little difference. As Eliot et al. (2021) concluded their very broad review of the research literature

 Despite clear behavioral differences between men and women, s/g differences in the brain are small and inconsistent, once individual brain size is accounted for. … the present synthesis indicates that such “real” or universal sex-related difference do not exist. Or at best, they are so small as to be buried under other sources of individual variance arising from countless genetic, epigenetic, and experiential factors. Thus, s/g differences in brain architecture may be similar to sex effects in gene-phenotype architecture; while statistically discernable in a very large (>100,000) sample, such effects contributed only 1.4 % to the accuracy of genotype-phenotype prediction...In layperson’s terms, these findings can be interpreted as rebutting popular discourse about the “male brain” and “female brain” as distinct organs." (p. 690)




References

Blow, N. (2009, April 16). Functional neuroscience: How to get ahead in imaging. Nature, 458, 925-928. Available online at http://www.nature.com/nature/journal/v458/n7240/full/458925a.html

"Advances in magnetic resonance imaging are helping sciences learn more about the structure and function of the brain. Nathan Blow looks at how far the technology has developed and where it could go." (site blurb)

Deisseroth, K. (2015). Optogenetics: 10 years of microbial opsins in neuroscience. Nature Neuroscience, 18,(9), 1213-1225.

DeWitt, I., & Rauschecker, J. P. (2012). Phoneme and word recognition in the auditory ventral stream. PNAS, published online before print, February 1, 2012. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1113427109

Eliot, L., Ahmed, A., Khan, H., & Patel, J. (2021) Dump the “dimorphism”: Comprehensive synthesis of human brain studies reveals few male-female differences beyond size. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 125, 667-697. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.02.026

Liu, S., Seidlitz, J., Blumenthal, J. D., Clasen, L. S., & Razhahan, A. (2020). Intergrative structural, functional, and transcriptomic analyses of sex-biased brain organization in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1919091117

Rauschecker, J. P., & Scott, S. K. (2009). Maps and streams in the auditory cortex: Nonhuman primates illuminate human speech processing. Nature Neuroscience, 12(6), 718-724. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2331

Rennels, J. L., & Cummings, A. J. (2013). Sex differences in facial scanning - Similarities and dissimilarities between infants and adults. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 37(2), 111-117. https://doi.org/10.1177/0165025412472411

Schmolesky, M. (2007). The primary visual cortex. Accessed February 11, 2023 from https://webvision.med.utah.edu/book/part-ix-brain-visual-areas/the-primary-visual-cortex/

Shapson-Coe, A., Januszewski, M., Berger, D. R., Pope, A., Wu, Y., Blakely, T., ... & Lichtman, J. W. (2021). A connectomic study of a petascale fragment of human cerebral cortex. BioRxiv, 2021-05. https://www.biorxiv.org/content/10.1101/2021.05.29.446289.full.pdf

This page was first posted February 3, 2005